知觉学习帮助成人弱视提高视力
2013-04-13 17:59:18 来源: 多宝视
拥有一定视觉任务的训练,也就是称为“知觉学习”的过程,是可以使表现得到明显的提高。在成人,对于基本的刺激如局部朝向、视网膜定位和代表眼具有特异性,说明了位于初级视皮层的神经处理的修正。然而这些低水平的学习是否影响如识别这样的高水平的视觉任务还不清楚。通过对在“关键期”初级视皮层发育异常(导致弱视)而造成功能障碍的成人视觉系统进行低水平的训练,我们在这里证明了低水平的改变可能产生转移至高级视觉任务,在知觉方面的明显改善。这个训练过程结果表现出在对比敏感度和字母识别任务方面的两倍的提高。这些发现证明了知觉学习能很有效地提高成人视觉系统中在关键期没有得到发展的基本视功能的表现。
弱视表现为多种空间视觉功能的异常,包括视敏度、对比敏感度功能、和游标视敏度的降低,同样也表现出空间扭曲、异常的空间交互、轮廓识别的障碍。另外,弱视患者还存在双眼视异常比如立体视功能障碍和双眼总和异常。在生命的前10年后(10~12)视觉障碍常是不可逆的,在这个时候是视觉发育成熟的终止时期。视力的降低被认为是初级视皮层的神经元网络的异常处理的结果,特别是方向选择性神经元和他们的联系的处理异常。在这个研究中描述的知觉学习程序是通过有效的刺激这些神经元和有效的改善他们的空间交互作用来训练这些网络。
在人类视觉中的空间交互作用可以通过探察有侧方目标存在时的局部目标的对比度来检查。这些实验说明当有方向一致并且排成行的高对比度的Gabor(GS)信号位于侧面时,中心GS的对比度阈值降低。兴奋性的交互作用是距离依赖性的,当侧面信号大约在3倍于GS波长时达到最大。小的分离能提高目标的阈值,这依赖于侧面目标的对比度和相位。信号单位记录说明潜在的机制位于初级视皮层。
初级视皮层的神经反应受到位置、朝向和空间频率的调制。近来关于猫和猴的研究证据表明初级视皮层的神经反映受到远距离图象的调制,观察到既有兴奋作用也有抑制作用,这依赖与刺激的对比度和结构。心理物理学和电生理的结果表明弱视患者有异常交互作用,存在抑制范围增加。
可能在早期发育阶段弱视视觉系统异常的视觉信息输入导致了视皮层活动形态的扭曲,从而引起联接的发育异常。
方法
这里的结果来自随机、盲法对照研究。包括病人有斜视性和屈光参差性弱视,年龄在9~55岁(常没有接受过治疗)。弱视眼最佳矫正视力测量在6/9到6/30,用糖尿病视网膜病变早期治疗研究视力表,ETDRS。每一个受试者(或父母/法定监护人)均签知情同意书,并经过Institutional Review Board的认可。总共77名弱视病人(分为2组)和16名正常视力受试者(对照组)参加这次研究。
他们的临床资料和治疗史在表1和图2中描述。最佳矫正视力是通过三个随机选择的ETDRS视力表来检测([LogMAR (最小分辨角的对数) 视力表],这是由对受检者分配的亚组是不知道的临床医生来进行.每个研究中参加者在2~4周中每个治疗节为30分钟,总共45 ±15个节(平均标准差)。
每个患者均进行一系列全面的评估,包括与弱视相关因素的具体眼病史,如弱视诊断年龄、治疗史、家族史等。也进行了详细的眼科检查,包括睫状体麻痹散瞳验光。眼球运动同样也检查,通过遮盖试验检查眼球位置,通过Worth4点法和Titmus立体图检测双眼视觉功能。根据收集的基础数据,弱视的类型被详细分类。CSF以基线检测,它是在治疗过程中和治疗后随访中进行检查,是通过3m远的一个贴在墙上的表实现(S.W.C.T.,Stereo Optical Company, Chicago)。室内照度被人为控制(≈140cd/m,人为控制在68~240cd/m)。这些条栅在所有频率的视角都在1.4°。用于治疗的是空间频率在1.5~12周/度的灰度条栅(GS)刺激,并受到背景灰度为40cd/m2调制。在所有的试验中GS的标准差等于其波长(σ=λ)。刺激呈现在一台Philips107P多媒体显示器上,显示器屏幕的有效大小为24·32cm,在150cm的地方观看,视角为9×12°。受试者在一间小暗室里,周围只有来至于屏幕的光照.。
对比度阈值是通过要求受试这二选一的方法来测量(二项迫选法)。目标呈现在2副图之一中,每个80~320ms,间隔500ms。受试者坐在离显示器1.5米的地方并戴上矫正眼镜的同时遮盖非弱视眼,任务是察觉目标,目标在图像两次闪现中的其中一次出现。在两次呈现之间是一个可以看见的注视点表示目标的位置。受试者自己控制速度来呈现每一个图象。在每一对图象出现后如果给出了错误的回答就会给予一个声音反馈。标准的治疗节包括一个GS对比度察觉任务,任务中可能有或没有共线性高对比的侧翼斑。对比度任务阈值的测量是通过3下1上的阶梯法(每一步为0.1对数单位),以在79%正确率时估计刺激强度。
在开始训练前,两个节被用于测量基础的空间功能,如对比敏感度和空间交互,后者代表皮层抑制和易化的程度。
我们在这些节中选出最初训练参数;开始的空间频率和总体方向设为最高空间频率,这时总体方向最差,对比度阈值比正常值的两倍还低,而不是大于15%。在训练期,刺激空间频率和方向则变化,开始于低空间频率(按照上面的标准设置)而逐渐向高频率变化,每个空间频率有四个方向。
接下来的治疗节中通过自动的电脑决策的参数而个体化,依赖于前面治疗阶段中的表现(跟没有弱视的受试者的标准表现水平相关)。治疗节包括一个侧向交互任务的训练,这个任务中包括对不同参数的独立GS斑的察觉。一个标准的治疗节包括了存在不同目标-侧翼分离距离的10-15段。在每个治疗节中空间频率和方向是持续的。上面提到的训练程序是用于治疗组中的所有弱视患者。在第2治疗组中在同样的流程下训练,但是开始的空间频率比第一组要低八度(见表1)。对照组给予相似的任务,同样要求察觉GS目标。作为第一个治疗组的10个受试者,刺激的分布如对比度(高)和空间频率(低)在训练节中是固定和保持不变的。后来这些受试者都达到了很好的能力。作为第二个治疗组对照的另4个受试者,目标的特性被与该治疗组一样的同一流程所调整。其中2个给予没有侧翼的低对比度目标,另2个有侧翼训练,但空间频率和方向在高对比度下调整。
根据第1组的协议规定,当经过连续12个连续治疗节后视力没有提高则治疗终止,在第1个对照组9个受试者参加了12个治疗节,1个受试者参加了16个治疗节。在第2组,规则同协议不一样,4个受试者中的3个分别连续进行了20。24,28个治疗节。在这治疗组中患者视力提高,而且他们的治疗持续了45±15治疗节(平均SD)。
结果研究结果证明弱视者的连接不足。侧向掩蔽曲线显示在图2a,是弱视眼(已矫正)视力与正常者的比较。受试者个体差异由三个典型病人显示出来(S1-S3)。展示两组弱视者的结果,最初的空间频率的设定符合两种不同的敏感度标准。
第一治疗组(n =40 of 44,开口方形)最初用3到12cpd的一系列空间频率进行检测(mean = 5.9, SD = 2.8 across participants),第二治疗组(n = 19;三角形)用1.5到6cpd低范围空间频率测试(mean = 2.7, SD = 1.6),第二组正常者(n = 16)空间频率的范围根第一治疗组一样(3–12 cpd; mean = 7.7, SD = 3.6)。数据清晰显示出弱视者易化作用的缺失,跟正常(第一弱视组)相比前者在高空间频率的抑制范围更大。这种抑制作用是我们联想到弱视者典型的拥挤现象。
低空间频率(第二弱视组)的结果显示接近于正常的易化作用,与弱视者在低空间频率刺激下的视力正常是一致的。其次,个人阈值升高,平均范围是2-6,被用于量化易化作用,其分布显示在图2b中。弱视第一组的平均阈值(均数+标准差)升高是0.18 +- 0.1对数单位(抑制, n = 40),弱视第二组是-0.11 +- 0.02(易化, n = 19),正常组是-0.12 +- 0.03。这些结果显示弱视者的空间交互异常与敏感度的降低是相关的,而且能解释这里和别处观察到的一些个体差异。
这里让我们特别感兴趣的是有散光的弱视亚组。在这组中,眼的光学畸变不是等方性的,但是在一个轴向上显示更模糊。对已矫正眼用不同朝向探索弱视者的侧向交互作用。用侧翼(实心红线)和不用(蓝色虚线)侧翼,目标阈值在图2c中显示出来,测试散光在10度方向,中心-侧翼距离时3倍,4个朝向的弱视患者的阈值。阈值显示沿散光轴(最大扭曲度,柱镜屈光力)抑制作用很强,而其正交轴接近正常。因为在发育阶段,屈光参差发展比较延迟,可能在屈光稳定以后,结果显示侧向交互作用的异常和对弱视脑发育的压力联系很紧密。
知觉学习不只是在青少年期通过改善视觉功能,有证据显示通过训练改变成人的视皮层。一些研究指出成人空间交互的可塑性是来自重复实践中央-侧翼任务实现。正常受试者兴奋性交互作用的增加,而且抑制性交互作用的降低(程度较小),这也见于对猴子的研究。在学习的研究中观察到的高刺激敏感性指出视皮层可塑性是任务依赖性的,训练中特定的连接被激活去修饰改善视功能。我们这里显示用一大系列适宜参数刺激,定位皮层大范围的交互作用,可能改善成人弱视不发达的空间交互能力。此外,已经获得连接性改善的弱视患者标准视力表视力得到改善。
标准训练节是察觉在有或没有侧翼共线高对比度斑时小的条栅条纹的对比度(a GS; see Fig. 1),在训练的过程中(达到80次),改变大小(空间频率)和刺激方向,从低空间频率开始逐渐增至高频率,每个空间频率有4个方向。另外,在训练前或后用标准对比敏感度表测量弱视者对比敏感度。比正常者表现出高阈值(低敏感性),在低空间频率时在正常低限范围内(跟正常没有明显不同),在高空间频率时显示高敏感度缺失。训练的结果是所有空间频率下敏感度提高,在高空间频率范围内提高到正常范围内。(图3a)
CSF曲线在1.5, 3, 6, 12, and 18 cpd的空间频率,每个频率下阈值分别提高2.21, 2.12, 2.93,4.23, and 2.05(紫线,治疗后12个月),均达到统计学差异。此外,如图2显示的,侧向抑制作用在训练后显著降低,如图3b(22个弱视患者,第二组,6cpd),在学习后显示没有抑制(提高0.15个对数单位,40%)。易化的提高(抑制的降低)和VA之间的相关系数r是0。68。意味着侧向易化的提高能够解释VA增加了46%。
VA,相似于字母辨别,要求受试者把图形映射成已知类型。因为在视皮层的空间滤过器滤过任何视觉信息的输入,估计也限制字母分辨的能力。特别地,上面描述的侧向抑制的增加会降低字母分辨性(拥挤效应),就像在标准VA测试中,当要辨认的字母周围有其他字母时。因此,可以预估训练引起的抑制交互作用的降低能改善视力。这种预计通过训练期间测试者常规的视力测试加以证实。图4a显示3个病人的数据,显示28次训练后,空间分辨率得到显著提高,达到了正常的视力表现(如小于等于6/7.6, Snellen equivalent)。控制组(安慰剂)用高对比度视标,并没有侧翼训练,显示没有提高。在图4b中我们展示治疗组(63人)和控制组(14人)在4个训练阶段的间隔期的组视力分数。
相对于初始阈值,根据空间分辨率的增加是有所记录的。联合治疗组在头8次训练后快速提高35%,随后相对缓慢的学习速度,在训练48次后达到78%的增加(0.25 log units)。没有用侧翼视标训练的控制组分数稳定。用高对比度视标和侧翼进行训练的两个控制组视力没有提高。在12次训练(第一控制组的最后一次试验)后作两个独立组的t-检验,显示对于相应治疗组的控制组受试者得出的结果是_0.2% (P _ 0.002),可能是偶然的。当考虑治疗的病人时,在第一治疗组44人中只有2人(4.5%有_0.05 LogMAR (0.5 ETDRS lines)的提高,而第一控制组10人中有7人(70%)在这范围内。控制组有一人提高了0.08 LogMAR,有两人提高了0.06 LogMAR。在12次训练后,对两个治疗组(63人)平均提高0.15 _ 0.01 (mean _ SE) LogMAR,而控制组(14人)平均提高0.01 _ 0.02,这差别很明显(独立组t-检验,P _ 0.0001)。3个控制组受试者(第二组)再训练12次。治疗组的病人在12次到24次中显著提高30%(from 0.15 _ 0.01 to 0.2 _ 0.01),三个控制组受试者几乎没有提高(from 0.03 _ 0.04 to 0.02 _ 0.04)。
这种观察到的治疗组和对照组间明显的区别说明了重复的视力检查并不会引起视力的提高。同样,依赖于对视标特征的皮层学习(目标对比度、侧翼的出现)的结果说明了并不仅是训练本身引起的能力的提高。总的来说,两个治疗组的视力平均提高78%(中间的为66%),尽管结果表现出相对较大的分散度7~307%,治疗后的最好矫正视力达到了0.16±0.02(好于6/9)。43个病人68%)得到了2行或更多的ETDRS的提高,48个病人中有40个病人(83%)在治疗前差于6/12治疗后好于6/12。视力的提高对年龄的依赖性并不明显(P=0.66),表明了可塑性不受年龄的限制,或弱视类型的限制(斜视性或屈光参差性;P=0.55),但和最初的缺损有一定相关性(P<0.05)。在对照组的受试验者表明了相对独立的结果:比最初视力统一增加了13%~20%
讨论
这个研究的结果为成年弱视的皮质可塑性提供了有力的证据。以前的研究已经用“心理解剖”方法来检测学习基本视觉任务过程中皮质调制的潜在变化,这是通过测量对低水平视觉特征的学习特异性来实现。目前这方法是针对没有在发育时而获得的功能的重建。我们在这里证明了弱视空间整合的扭曲模式是发育依赖性的,而且和光学畸变有关(如在子午线性弱视)因为在多数的弱视眼(知道被诊断)光学扭曲在关键期是没有矫正的,从弱视眼到皮质的输入是异常的,导致了视皮层发育的异常。通过我们的患者训练后侧向抑制的减弱能归因于恢复了发生在早期可塑期的发育损害。可以作为另一选择的是,如果弱视和阻碍低水平视觉功能发育的感官缺损有关,我们的结果可能说明:训练在恢复这些功能方面是成功的,尽管并不需要在关键期受影响的相同神经元网络内进行训练。
知觉学习是刺激和任务特异性的。这些特征常用于推测发生学习的解剖位置。在正常视力的成年人和成年的弱视病人都表现出训练后游标视力的提高。 尽管那些研究通过应用相似的刺激(线条和字母)证明了重复的游标视力任务能提高另外任务(同样受到视力的限制)的能力,我们证明了通过与视力检查(字母)不同的刺激(GSs)来训练一些很不同的和功能性的更基础的任务(对比度察觉)。这种进步可能是训练刺激物设置的多维性的结果;它的作用是提高了视觉系统早期处理过程并最后提高所有依赖于低水平视觉表现质量的所有高水平处理。视觉功能进步的维持说明了学习不只是暂时的适应效应而是一种长期持续的视觉皮质的改变,这是与以前的知觉学习研究是一致的。
另外,在非训练任务(如视力)的提高排除了由于训练任务的特别性的训练效应引起提高的可能性。这个研究说明了成年弱视者对比度检测阈值的进步。这种进步只在治疗组获得,他们都是训练的具有侧翼的信号,然而对照组 (n =14)在没有侧翼的高和低对比度的GS训练时没有一点进步。对于象调节和眼球运动这样的‘‘front-end’’资源可能是由我们的学习效应引起的假设是不太可能的。弱视眼的视力检查是在远视全矫的情况下进行的,这是通过睫状肌麻痹剂而实现的。而且,视力检查是在3m距离进行的,这时调节只有1/3屈光度,所以作用是很小的。我们的病人中多数是在调节幅度受到限制的年龄阶段(40岁后的弱视患者几乎就没有这种能力了)。注视功能的提高也不太能解释视力的提高,因为偏中心注视的病人是被排除了的。
在斜视性和屈光参差性弱视病人间没有明显的区别,而且大家知道后者受到注视问题的影响更小有趣的是,散光的病人,在某一方向进行刺激时常表现出好的视力而另一方向则不是其视觉表现也显示提高。另外,许多斜视患者的绝对对比度阈值是在正常范围内的排除了front-end效应的可能性,然而他们表现出侧向交互作用的缺损,这通过他们已经知道的知觉扭曲可以预测。斜视性弱视参加者也表现出训练中视力的提高。而且,如果调节、注视或两者都对视力提高有作用,那对照组的弱视病人也应该有同样提高,但情况并非如此。
有一个和现在的治疗有着同样理论基础的治疗是CAM疗法。这种设备与遮盖结合应用于儿童,他是一种旨在把视觉皮质暴露于不同的空间频率的具有黑百条纹的旋转盘。用对照实验来研究CAM刺激疗法的治疗作用发现在治疗组和对照组见没有明显区别。尽管两种方法(目前这种方法和CAM法)都是以神经生理学为基础的,但是他们在许多重要的方面是不同的。CAM治疗中对比度高(对照组也一样),然而我们的治疗组用的是低对比度的目标和高对比度的侧翼。在CAM治疗中,注意力集中在无关的任务中(绘画),然而在我们的实验中的病人是让其注意中心目标。在CAM治疗中,很多方向和空间频率在短期内出现,而不管弱视的程度或者进步的水平;在我们的研究中,一次实验只有一个方向和空间频率出现,个性化的刺激参数设置与受试者的进步相协调。而且,CAM中的周边运动的刺激可能已经干扰了受试者注意中心刺激,然而我们的方法提供了一种有效的和选择性的中心刺激。
知觉学习有望成为一种治疗成人弱视的主流方法。在年轻的儿童,弱视可以通过早期的遮盖来治疗,这样通过遮盖视力较好眼强迫弱视眼发挥作用。通过延长遮盖好眼的治疗被认为在成年弱视是不切实际的。在治疗中遮盖的作用是与年龄呈负相关的。在11~20岁的人群失败的几率是0~3岁儿童的7.9倍。治疗的成功率有点难估计,因为缺乏公认的定义,但通常在年轻儿童是60%~70%,可以同这里发现的成人成功率比较。这个结果支持使用一个构造的方法来提高成年弱视的视力,目的在于针对弱视者的特别缺损。这里的知觉学习方法有可能可以推广应用与其他的感觉或非感觉的由于发育问题造成的大脑模块。
Improving Reading Speed for People with Central Vision Loss through Perceptual Learning(原文PDF下载)Author Affiliations
From the School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, California.
Corresponding author: Susana T. L. Chung, 360 Minor Hall, #2020 Berkeley, CA 94720-2020; s.chung@berkeley.edu.
Abstract
Purpose. Perceptual learning has been shown to be effective in improving visual functions in the normal adult visual system, as well as in adults with amblyopia. In this study, the feasibility of applying perceptual learning to enhance reading speed in people with long-standing central vision loss was evaluated.
Methods. Six observers (mean age, 73.8) with long-standing central vision loss practiced an oral sentence-reading task, with words presented sequentially using rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP). A pre-test consisted of measurements of visual acuities, RSVP reading speeds for six print sizes, the location of the preferred retinal locus for fixation (fPRL), and fixation stability. Training consisted of six weekly sessions of RSVP reading, with 300 sentences presented per session. A post-test, identical with the pre-test, followed the training.
Results. All observers showed improved RSVP reading speed after training. The improvement averaged 53% (range, 34–70%). Comparisons of pre- and post-test measurements revealed little changes in visual acuity, critical print size, location of the fPRL, and fixation stability.
Conclusions. The specificity of the learning effect, and the lack of changes to the fPRL location and fixation stability suggest that the improvements are not due to observers adopting a retinal location with better visual capability, or an improvement in fixation. Rather, the improvements are likely to represent genuine plasticity of the visual system despite the older ages of the observers, coupled with long-standing sensory deficits. Perceptual learning might be an effective way of enhancing visual performance for people with central vision loss.
Previous SectionNext Section
Introduction
Reading is difficult and slow for many low vision patients, especially those with central vision loss who are obligated to use their peripheral retina to read. The leading cause of visual impairment in developed countries is age-related macular degeneration (AMD),1–3 which is also the leading cause of central vision loss. Because reading is the most common clinical complaint as well as the primary goal for patients with central vision loss seeking visual rehabilitation,1,4,5 improving the reading performance for these patients is a key challenge facing low vision rehabilitation.
Previous studies have examined a number of ways to improve reading performance in people with central vision loss. For instance, in low vision clinics, patients are routinely prescribed with magnifiers for reading tasks. However, even with magnification, reading speed in people with central vision loss is still lower than that at the normal fovea.5–8 Substantial effort has been invested to determine the mode of text presentation that offers people with central vision loss the fastest reading speed, including page format, scrolling-text in the horizontal or the vertical direction, and rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP), where words are presented one at a time on a display. Most studies found no significant differences in reading speed for different text presentation modes for people with central vision loss.9–11 A handful of studies found a small advantage of using RSVP,12 especially if the word presentation rate varied with word length13 or when observers were allowed to adjust their own presentation rate.14 Other attempts have explored whether simple manipulation of text typography and typesetting such as increasing letter spacing15,16 and line spacing,17 which presumably reduces the crowding effect among text, could improve reading speed. Unfortunately, none of these simple manipulations of text typography or typesetting improve reading speed for people with central vision loss.16,17
In this study, I explored the feasibility of using perceptual learning, a method that has proven to be effective in improving visual functions in normal and amblyopic visual systems, to improve reading speed for people with central vision loss. Perceptual learning is defined as “any relatively permanent and consistent change in the perception of a stimulus array, after practice or experience with this array”.18 Practically, perceptual learning is synonymous with “training” or “practice.”19 Previous studies have shown that visual performance improves with practice for a variety of tasks,19–25 in younger as well as in older adults,26,27 and in the normal fovea and periphery alike.19,27–31 In addition, perceptual learning has also shown effectiveness in improving visual functions in adults with amblyopia (monocular sensory loss of vision in the absence of an organic origin).32–38 In many cases, adults with amblyopia improved not only on the trained task, but their visual acuities (an untrained task) also improved as a result of training.33–37 Considering the effectiveness of perceptual learning in improving visual functions in the normal visual system and in adults with amblyopia, I asked whether perceptual learning would also be effective in improving reading performance for people with central vision loss. Clearly, there are many challenges facing the use of perceptual learning in improving visual functions in people with central vision loss. Specifically, the most common cause of central vision loss is AMD,1–3 which primarily afflicts people older than 65 years of age. It is well known that even though visual performance of older adults can improve with practice, more training may be required before the improvement reaches a plateau26 and that there may be more day-to-day lapses in improvement, which would lead to an overall reduction in the amount of learning.27 Also, in contrast to amblyopia, the majority of people with central vision loss suffer from bilateral vision loss and their functioning retina may not be healthy; whether these would impact the effectiveness of perceptual learning for people with central vision loss is unknown. Hence, despite the promising benefits that perceptual learning can deliver, it remains unclear if people with central vision loss can benefit from it. To my knowledge, there exists no published paper on using perceptual learning to improve visual functions in people with central vision loss, although previous studies have examined whether or not reading performance could be improved by training comprehension,39 or training patients to use a CCTV or stand magnifier to read.40,41 Comprehension training is a cognitive task, and the use of a CCTV or stand magnifier requires motor skills, making it unclear that any improvement from these training represents genuine improvement in the sensory system, which is the basis of perceptual learning. The goal of this study was to determine the feasibility of using perceptual learning to improve reading speed for people with central vision loss.
Previous works have established that reading performance in the normal periphery benefits from perceptual learning based on the following training tasks: identifying random sequences of three letters at various positions across the visual field,19,27,31 performing a lexical decision task,31 and reading.31 The greatest improvement in reading speed was obtained using reading as the training task.31 Consequently, reading was used as the training task in this study.
Previous SectionNext Section
Methods
Six observers with central vision loss, including four with AMD and two with Stargardt disease, participated in this study. All had long-standing central vision loss (minimum 7 years, see Table 1). Entering visual acuities and other characteristics of the observers are given in Table 1. Although all were avid readers or had the need to read constantly before their vision loss, all observers had stopped reading on a regular basis long before their participation in this study because of their visual deficits. Subjects (S)1 through S3 had previously participated in another study in the laboratory (2–3 hours) in which RSVP was used to measure how reading speed changed with some text manipulation; while S4–S6 had no prior exposure to RSVP reading. All observers gave written informed consent before the commencement of data collection. This research followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Committee for Protection of Human Subjects at the University of California, Berkeley.
The basic experimental design consisted of a pre-test, six sessions of training, followed by a post-test. The pre-test consisted of measurements of monocular visual acuities, the location of the preferred retinal locus for fixation (fPRL) and fixation stability, and measurements of reading speed as a function of print size using the rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) paradigm. Details of these procedures are given below. From the reading speed versus print size data obtained at the pre-test, the critical print size, the smallest print size at which maximum reading speed could still be attained, was determined.42 The print size used for training corresponded to 1.4× the critical print size.19 For S1–S5, training consisted of six weekly sessions of RSVP reading. It was shown previously that in the normal periphery, the improvement after perceptual learning was not different whether observers were trained on a daily or a weekly basis,43 justifying my choice of a weekly training schedule in this study, which was more convenient for observers with central vision loss. In each session, observers read a total of 300 sentences, divided into 10 blocks of 30 trials each. Reading speed was determined for each block of 30 trials (average number of words presented per block = 326.3 ± 8.7 [SD]). S6 could complete only seven blocks in the first training session, and eight in each of the subsequent sessions, for a total of 47 blocks over the course of six training sessions. Also, because of his work schedule and illness, he had a three-week gap between training sessions 3 and 4. The post-test, identical with the pre-test except that all measurements were conducted in reversed order, took place a week after the last training session.
Visual Acuity Measurements
Visual acuity was measured monocularly using the Bailey-Lovie high-contrast letter acuity chart.44 Acuities were scored on a letter-by-letter basis, with each letter scoring 0.02 log units.45 All observers were tested at a distance of 10 ft, except for S6 who was tested at 5 ft because of his poorer acuities.
Location of fPRL and Fixation Stability Measurements
The retinal locations used for fixation, and fixation stability of each observer were determined using a scanning laser ophthalmoscope (Rodenstock 101; Rodenstock, Munich, Germany). Observers were asked to look at the center of a fixation cross subtending 1° (2° for observer S6) using their preferred eye. Fundus images were captured continuously for 30 s at a frame rate of 30 Hz. Offline analyses were performed using custom-written software in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA), and included a frame-by-frame analysis of the retinal locations that the observer used to fixate the cross, and the variability of eye position from frame to frame. To quantify fixation stability, I adopted the conventional method of calculating the bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA, in deg2)46–49 over each trial of 30 s, which refers to the area of the retinal surface on which the fixation target was imaged 68% of the time. The BCEA value reported in this article, for each observer and for the pre- or post-test, represents the averaged values of two or three trials, collected a few minutes apart.
Reading Speed Measurements
Oral reading speed for single sentences was measured binocularly using the RSVP paradigm. The procedures were very similar to those used in previous studies.15,19,30,42 In brief, on each trial, a single sentence was chosen randomly from a pool of 2630 sentences. Each sentence contained between 8 and 14 words (mean, 10.9 ± 1.7 [SD]) and included only words that were among the 5000 most frequently used words in written English, according to word-frequency tables derived from the British National Corpus.50 Words were rendered in Times Roman font and were presented left-justified on the display, one word at a time in rapid succession, each for a fixed exposure duration. For each block of trials, the Method of Constant Stimuli was used to present sentences at five or six word exposure durations (five for all training sessions, five or six for pre-tests and post-tests depending on observers). The number of words read correctly was recorded for each sentence. A cumulative-Gaussian function was used to fit each set of data (based on 30–36 sentences presented) relating the percentage of words read correctly as a function of exposure duration, from which the reading speed based on the word exposure duration that yielded 80% of the words read correctly was derived.
For the pre- and post-tests, reading speeds were determined for six print sizes spanning 0.75 log units in range, for each observer. A two-line fit (on log-log axes) was used to fit each set of reading speed versus print size data,15,19,30,42 with the slope of the second line constrained to zero. The intersection of the two lines represents the critical print size. The height of the second line (slope = 0) gives the maximum reading speed.
Text stimuli were generated using a Visual Stimulus Generator graphics board (VSG 2/5; Cambridge Research Ltd, Rochester, UK) controlled by a workstation (Dell Precision 650; Dell, Austin, TX) and presented on a 24-inch color graphics display monitor (Model# GDM-FW900; Sony, New York, NY). The resolution of the display was 1280 × 960 pixels, at a frame rate of 80 Hz. The temporal dynamics of the display were verified using a photo-detector and an oscilloscope. Stimuli were black text (2 cd/m2) presented on a white background (144 cd/m2).
Previous SectionNext Section
Results
Reading speed (words per minute, wpm) measured using RSVP, is plotted as a function of training block in Figure 1 for each observer. All observers showed improved reading speed with training, although there was substantial individual observer variability. In particular, while the improvement of observer S1 reached a plateau after the first training session, observer S4 continued to improve over the course of the training. The change in reading speed with training can be described by an exponential function of the form (smooth line drawn through each set of data in Fig. 1): where y0 is the asymptotic reading speed with sufficient training, A is the maximum improvement in reading speed due to training and τ is the time constant. To quantify the improvement, the ratio of reading speed between the last and the first block of training was calculated based on the fitted values. Across the six observers, this ratio ranges between 1.34 and 1.70, with an average of 1.53. In other words, the average improvement in reading speed after six sessions of training was 53% (paired t-test on log reading speed: t(5) = 12.46, P < 0.0001).
An alternative way to quantify the improvement in reading speed due to training is to compare the maximum reading speed derived from the reading speed versus print size plots before and after training. Such plots are shown in Figure 2 for all six observers. The straight lines in each plot represent the two-line fit. The ratio of the maximum reading speed (the plateau of the two-line fit) after and before training averages 1.55, representing a significant improvement in reading speed (paired t-test on log reading speed: t(5) = 8.24, P = 0.0004). This improvement can also be visualized in Figure 3A, in which the pre- and post-maximum reading speeds are compared for all observers. The dashed line represents the 1:1 line, indicating no change in the maximum reading speed before and after training. All the data points lie above the 1:1 line, implying that the maximum reading speeds improved after training for all observers.
A characteristic of perceptual learning is its specificity.20,21,23,30 To examine if the training effect transfers to an improvement in critical print size and/or visual acuity, the pre- and post-test critical print size and visual acuities were compared in Figures 3B and 3D were compared. Clearly, all data points fall very close to the 1:1 line, implying that neither the critical print size (averaged post/pre ratio = 1.0; paired t(5) = 0.95, P = 0.38) nor the visual acuity (averaged post/pre ratio = 0.99; paired t(11) = 1.17, P = 0.27) changed after training.
To determine whether the improvement in reading speed was due to observers adopting a different PRL with better visual capabilities, or that observers learned to maintain steadier fixation, the location of the fPRL and fixation stability of each observer before and after training were compared. Figure 4 shows the fixation frequency distribution—the frequency distribution of the retinal locations used for fixation—superimposed on the fundus image, for each observer, before and after training. Clearly, the region over which most of the fixation occurred (roughly representing the fPRL), and the spread of the region, did not change substantially before and after training. To quantify the fixation stability, the conventional measurement of BCEA was adopted.46–49 Figure 3C shows that the BCEA (in deg2) of the observers was practically the same before and after training (paired t(5) = 0.13, P = 0.90), suggesting that the improvement in reading speed after training cannot be attributed to the adoption of a different PRL with better visual capabilities or the oculomotor system becoming more stable.
Discussion
After six weekly sessions of repeated training on an RSVP reading task, observers with central vision loss were able to improve their reading speeds by an average of 53%. This improvement did not transfer to visual acuity and critical print size measurements, implying that even though observers were able to read faster, they were not able to read smaller letters on an acuity chart or text of smaller sizes. The improvement cannot be attributed to a change in the location of the fPRL or better oculomotor control (steadier fixation). These results provide evidence for neural plasticity in human adults with long-standing central vision loss.
Neural plasticity in adults with sensory visual deficits is not at all a new concept. For over a decade, many studies have reported that human adults with amblyopia benefit from perceptual learning, in that many visual functions, including visual acuity, can improve through training.32–38 Because of the substantial benefits, perceptual learning has recently been proposed as a treatment for adult amblyopia.37,38 With respect to people with central vision loss, it is well known that after the loss of central vision, many of these individuals eventually adopt a retinal location (sometimes more than one) outside the afflicted macular area to serve as the PRL.51–58 This in itself is strong evidence that even for people with central vision loss, many of whom are elderly suffering from AMD, the visual cortex is still malleable and able to adapt to unfavorable visual experience. Therefore, it is not surprising that these people can benefit from perceptual learning.
Perceptual learning is known for its specificity,20,21,23,30 a characteristic that distinguishes itself from general practicing of a task. In this study, the specific improvements related to reading speed, but not to visual acuity or fixation stability, lend support to the argument that the observed improvements are indeed the consequence of perceptual learning, rather than to general training, the continued adaptation to the vision loss, or recent changes in central vision. Given the specificity of the improvements, an important and practical question that follows is whether the improvement in RSVP reading speed would generalize to the conventional page-reading task. Because page-reading requires more eye movements (inter-word saccades and return sweeps) than RSVP reading, it is possible that improvements in RSVP reading speed may not generalize to page-reading if eye-movement control is the primary limiting factor on page-reading. However, compared with people with intact central vision, patients with central vision loss do not benefit as much from RSVP reading, suggesting that the limitation of eye movements on reading may not be as important for people with central vision loss as for people with intact central fields. Further, there is evidence that RSVP and page-format reading show similar dependence on certain text parameters such as letter spacing.59 Therefore, it would be interesting to test in future studies whether improvements after RSVP training would transfer to page-reading, and whether RSVP training and page-reading training are equally effective in improving reading speed for patients with central vision loss.
Another characteristic of perceptual learning is the substantial individual observer variability with respect to the time course and the amount of improvement. In fact, it has been shown that up to 25–50% of observers failed to show improvement after training.29,60 In the present study, the six observers demonstrated variability in their time course of improvement (Fig. 1), arguing that a tailored amount of training for individual observers may be more appropriate than a one-size-fits-all approach. This is an important issue to resolve, not only from a scientific point of view, but also because the results may directly influence policy-makers in deciding on the number of training sessions that should be covered by health care plans, should perceptual learning be adopted as a rehabilitative option for patients with central vision loss.
Given these results, a logical question to ask is what underlies the improvements. A psychophysical approach to answer this question is to evaluate how observer performance is affected by the presence of different amount of external noise superimposed on the stimulus. The basis of this approach is to attribute the limitation in human performance to (1) the presence of internal noise in the visual system that limits the precision of perceptual responses and (2) the inability of the visual system to make full use of the information available in the stimulus.24,25,29,61,62 The mechanism underlying perceptual learning can then be inferred, by tracking how performance changes with different levels of external noise with time. In this study, because reading performance in the presence of external noise was not measured, the functional mechanism that underlies learning could not be inferred. However, based on previous studies for a variety of training tasks, the improvement after training is most likely due to the visual system being more capable of extracting the crucial information from the stimulus.24,25,29,61,62
A few caveats should be kept in mind while evaluating the interpretations presented here. First, I showed that the location of the fPRL did not change substantially after training. Because people with central vision loss can adopt different PRLs for different tasks,51–58 the data presented in this study cannot convincingly rule out the possibility that observers adopt an alternative retinal location that has better visual capability for the reading task. Currently, a larger-scale perceptual learning study is being designed. It will include the PRL for reading as a pre-post comparison measurement. Note that the measurements of fPRL were made monocularly, while the training was performed binocularly; therefore, it is also plausible that the characteristics of the binocular PRL could have changed as a result of training. Unfortunately, there is currently no known method to measure the binocular PRL. Second, considering that this was a feasibility study, a no-training control group was not included. However, it is easy to envision that a no-training control group is unlikely to show improvement because in real life, patients with central vision loss almost never show improvement in reading performance without practicing the use of their residual vision. A rough estimate of the improvement shown by a no-training control group based on simply performing the RSVP reading task twice (pre- and post-test) was 7–10%, as reported in previous studies.19,31 Considering that observers who receive training spend more time in the laboratory than those who do not receive any training, to control for the time spent in the laboratory, or the amount of interaction with laboratory personnel and the attention received, a better control design is to train another group of observers on a task that is unrelated to the training task being studied. In a previous study,31 three groups of normally sighted observers were trained on three different psychophysical tasks — lexical decision, trigram letter recognition, and RSVP reading, for a similar amount of time. A fourth group of observers did not receive any training. The group that was trained on RSVP reading yielded the largest magnitude of improvement (averaged 72%) while the improvements for the other two training groups averaged 40–50%, with the improvement for the no-training group being 10%. The differential magnitudes of improvement for the different training groups confirm that the improvements after training on an RSVP task are not due to psychological or general improvements because the observers spent long periods of time in the laboratory or because they received lots of attention from the laboratory personnel. Also, the largest magnitude of improvement exhibited by the RSVP training group is consistent with the specificity of perceptual learning,20,21,23,30 rather than just an improvement due to general training on any psychophysical tasks.
Clearly, if perceptual learning is to be used clinically to benefit patients with central vision loss, other issues have to be considered. For example, does the improvement after RSVP training transfer to a real-life page-reading task that involves a higher oculomotor demand? Will other modes of reading such as large-print page-reading be equally effective as a training task? Should the amount of training be tailored to each individual observer instead of giving the same amount of training to all observers? What is the optimal training duration? Does the improvement in reading speed also help improve the comprehension of the reading materials? These are all interesting questions which will be addressed in future studies.